The parallel trends assumption is crucial for difference-in-differences estimation, a method used to measure causal effects in observational studies. It states that without treatment, the average outcomes for treatment and control groups would follow parallel paths over time.
This assumption allows researchers to attribute differences in outcomes between groups to the treatment itself. Violating this assumption can lead to biased estimates and incorrect conclusions about causal effects, making it essential to carefully assess and address potential violations.
Definition of parallel trends assumption
- Fundamental assumption in difference-in-differences (DiD) estimation, a popular method for estimating causal effects in observational studies
- Requires that in the absence of treatment, the average outcomes for the treatment and control groups would have followed parallel paths over time
- Implies that any differences in outcomes between the two groups after treatment can be attributed to the causal effect of the treatment itself
Importance in difference-in-differences estimation
- Crucial for the validity of DiD estimates as it allows for the isolation of the causal effect of interest
- Enables the control group to serve as a valid counterfactual for the treatment group, representing what would have happened to the treatment group in the absence of treatment
- Violation of this assumption can lead to biased estimates and incorrect conclusions about the causal effect of the treatment
Conditions for parallel trends assumption
Treatment vs control groups
- Requires that the treatment and control groups are similar in terms of their pre-treatment characteristics and trends
- Groups should be affected by the same external factors and shocks over time
- Differences between the groups should be stable and not vary systematically with the treatment
Pre-treatment trends
- Assumption is more plausible when the treatment and control groups exhibit similar trends in the outcome variable prior to the treatment
- Parallel pre-treatment trends suggest that the groups would have continued to follow similar paths in the absence of treatment
- Divergence in pre-treatment trends raises concerns about the validity of the assumption
Absence of time-varying confounding
- Requires that there are no unobserved factors that affect the outcome variable differently for the treatment and control groups over time
- Time-varying confounders can cause the groups to diverge even in the absence of treatment, violating the parallel trends assumption
- Presence of such confounders can lead to biased estimates of the treatment effect
Assessing the validity of parallel trends assumption
Visual inspection of pre-treatment trends
- Plotting the outcome variable for the treatment and control groups over time can provide a visual assessment of the parallel trends assumption
- Similar pre-treatment trends suggest that the assumption is plausible, while diverging trends raise concerns
- Visual inspection should be supplemented with statistical tests for more rigorous evaluation
Placebo tests
- Involve artificially assigning the treatment to a different time period or group where no effect is expected
- If the parallel trends assumption holds, placebo tests should yield estimates close to zero
- Significant placebo effects suggest that the assumption may be violated and the DiD estimates may be biased
Covariate balance tests
- Check whether the treatment and control groups are balanced in terms of observable characteristics before and after the treatment
- Significant differences in covariates over time can indicate the presence of time-varying confounding and violation of the parallel trends assumption
- Covariate balance tests help assess the comparability of the groups and the plausibility of the assumption
Consequences of violating parallel trends assumption
Biased causal effect estimates
- Violation of the parallel trends assumption can lead to biased estimates of the treatment effect
- If the treatment and control groups would have followed different paths even in the absence of treatment, the DiD estimate will capture not only the true treatment effect but also the difference in underlying trends
- Bias can be positive or negative, depending on the direction of the violation
Misinterpretation of results
- Biased estimates can lead to incorrect conclusions about the effectiveness of the treatment
- Policymakers and researchers may attribute changes in outcomes to the treatment when they are actually driven by other factors
- Misinterpretation of results can have serious consequences for policy decisions and future research directions
Strategies for addressing violations
Including covariates
- Adding relevant covariates to the DiD model can help control for observable time-varying confounders
- Covariates should be selected based on theoretical considerations and data availability
- Including covariates can reduce bias and improve the plausibility of the parallel trends assumption
Synthetic control methods
- Involve constructing a synthetic control group as a weighted combination of untreated units that closely resembles the treatment group in terms of pre-treatment characteristics and trends
- Synthetic control methods can help address violations of the parallel trends assumption by creating a more suitable comparison group
- Requires a sufficient number of untreated units and careful selection of weighting variables
Triple difference estimators
- Extend the DiD approach by adding a third difference, such as a comparison group that is unaffected by the treatment
- Triple difference estimators can help control for time-varying confounders that affect both the treatment and control groups
- Requires the identification of a suitable third comparison group and additional assumptions about the nature of the confounding
Limitations of parallel trends assumption
Unobserved time-varying confounders
- Parallel trends assumption can be violated by the presence of unobserved factors that affect the treatment and control groups differently over time
- Such confounders are often difficult to measure or control for in observational studies
- Unobserved time-varying confounders can lead to biased estimates even if the parallel trends assumption appears to hold based on observable characteristics
Anticipation effects
- Occur when individuals or entities change their behavior in anticipation of the treatment, even before it is implemented
- Anticipation effects can cause the treatment and control groups to diverge prior to the actual treatment, violating the parallel trends assumption
- Ignoring anticipation effects can lead to biased estimates of the treatment effect
Functional form misspecification
- Parallel trends assumption is often tested and assessed using linear models, such as linear regression
- If the true relationship between the outcome and time is non-linear, the assumption may appear to hold even if it is actually violated
- Misspecification of the functional form can lead to incorrect conclusions about the validity of the parallel trends assumption and the causal effect of the treatment
Alternatives to difference-in-differences
Regression discontinuity designs
- Exploit a discontinuity in treatment assignment based on a continuous variable (running variable)
- Compares outcomes for units just above and below a cutoff value of the running variable
- Relies on the assumption that units near the cutoff are similar in terms of unobservable characteristics
Instrumental variables
- Use an exogenous source of variation (instrument) that affects the treatment but not the outcome directly
- Instrument should be correlated with the treatment and uncorrelated with unobserved confounders
- Allows for the estimation of causal effects in the presence of unmeasured confounding
Matching methods
- Involve pairing treated units with similar untreated units based on observable characteristics
- Matching methods aim to create a balanced sample that mimics a randomized experiment
- Common matching techniques include propensity score matching and coarsened exact matching